Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter February 1, 2014

Peer Effects and Policy: The Relationship between Classroom Gender Composition and Student Achievement in Early Elementary School

  • Michael A. Gottfried and Jennifer Graves EMAIL logo

Abstract

The existence of gender peer effects has been well-documented, yet regarding estimates that are best-suited for policy formation, the literature finds somewhat mixed results. This article builds on the gender peer effects literature in a number of ways. First, we focus on early elementary school students, for which fewer studies exist. We also test whether effects in early elementary grades are subject-specific. Contrary to findings for older grade levels, we find that estimates by gender are subject-specific for the early elementary grades. Second, previous studies using similar estimation have focused on very different geographical areas, while this study makes use of nationally representative elementary school data for the United States. Third, we explore whether effects vary across grades, for which the existing literature finds mixed results. We find that the negative subject-specific effects of having a higher proportion of boys in the classroom increases in magnitude across grades, with insignificant effects in kindergarten, negative and significant by first grade, and larger negative and significant effects by third grade. Our findings suggest that a more balanced gender mix in the classroom is optimal for both reading and math comprehension for both boys and girls. However, regarding math performance, there is also suggestive evidence that a 100% separation of genders could improve girls’ math performance without consequences for boys’ math performance, motivating further research into single-gender subject-specific instruction.

JEL Classification: I20; I21; J16

Appendix

Table 1

Differences in the percentage of boys in the classroom across student, classroom, teacher and school characteristics

Percent of boys in the classroom when the characteristic in the row heading is “true”Percent of boys in the classroom when the characteristics in the row heading is “false”
NMeanSDNMeanSD
Binary characteristics (true or false)
Student
Black3,8080.510.1025,1760.510.10
Hispanic3,2500.510.1025,7340.510.10
Asian1,2850.500.0927,6990.510.10
Has a disability5,1820.510.1023,8020.510.10
At or below poverty threshold4,4010.510.1024,5830.510.10
English is primary language at home27,1110.510.101,8730.510.10
Classroom, Teacher, or School
Class is less than 21 students10,8630.510.1218,1540.500.09
Teacher is male8130.520.1028,2040.510.10
Teacher experience less than 13 years16,1410.510.1012,8760.510.10
Teacher is white26,0940.510.102,9230.500.10
School less than 149 students1,5560.490.1327,4610.510.10
School less than 10 percent minority10,9310.500.1018,0860.510.10
School is private6,3440.490.1122,6730.510.10
Table 2

Table 5, no private schools in sample

BoysGirls
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentileReading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
30–40%−2.82**0.18−1.91*−0.160.820.81−1.18−1.02
(1.4)(0.72)(1.08)(0.7)(1.73)(0.83)(1.33)(0.8)
40–50%−3.09**0.13−1.9*−0.030.430.72−2.09−1.44*
(1.36)(0.7)(1.04)(0.68)(1.66)(0.79)(1.28)(0.78)
50–60%−3.88***−0.26−2.04**−0.070.350.87−2.16*1.2
(1.36)(0.7)(1.04)(0.68)(1.66)(0.8)(1.28)(0.78)
60–70%−3.98***−0.58−1.640.220.71.06−2.51**−1.5*
(1.43)(0.75)(1.1)(0.72)(1.69)(0.81)(1.3)(0.8)
70–80%−4.52**−1.34−2.13−0.55−1.38−0.01−3.87***−1.76*
(1.82)(0.95)(1.37)(0.91)(1.96)(0.96)(1.48)(0.93)
n11,39711,39711,39711,39711,14911,14911,14911,149
R20.840.850.850.860.860.860.850.86
Table 3

Table 5, sample excludes all schools with at least one classroom with a percent boys in the class less than 20% or greater than 80%

BoysGirls
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentileReading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
30–40%−1.740.35−2.02**−0.36−0.480.84−1.160.11
(1.26)(0.64)(0.92)(0.59)(1.45)(0.72)(1.16)(0.77)
40–50%−2.2*0.18−2.03**−0.33−0.330.65−1.73−0.52
(1.23)(0.62)(0.89)(0.57)(1.43)(0.7)(1.15)(0.75)
50–60%−2.68**−0.06−1.87**−0.19−0.590.7−1.82−0.3
(1.24)(0.63)(0.89)(0.58)(1.43)(0.7)(1.15)(0.75)
60–70%−3.42***−0.43−1.89**−0.01−0.370.89−1.98*−0.5
(1.31)(0.67)(0.95)(0.62)(1.46)(0.72)(1.17)(0.77)
70–80%−4.5***−1.18−2.88**−0.98−2.62*−0.22−3.4**−0.81
(1.74)(0.88)(1.28)(0.85)(1.71)(0.85)(1.34)(0.88)
n14,29114,29114,29114,29114,21614,21614,21614,216
R20.850.850.860.860.860.860.850.86
Table 4

Classroom gender composition, sample broken out by gender, quadratic and cubic terms

Sample restricted to students in classrooms with greater than 20% boys and less than 80% boys
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
Sample: Boys
Percent of class, boys−10.78−18.864.1418.51−10.02−81.1*−0.66−33.85
(13.19)(56.19)(6.94)(28.26)(10.28)(43.88)(6.5)(26.61)
Percent of class, boys24.9921.92−6.48−36.588.73157.6*0.6770.18
(13.02)(113.88)(6.91)(57.73)(10.21)(89.33)(6.5)(54.7)
Percent of class, boys3−11.3420.17−99.75*−46.58
(75.02)(38.34)(58.96)(36.51)
Calculated Marginal Effect of Boys−5.79−5.45−2.34−2.94−1.291.690.011.40
Wald test on joint significance (p-value)0.010.030.060.130.480.230.990.65
n14,39814,39814,39814,39814,39814,39814,39814,398
R20.850.850.850.850.860.860.860.86
Sample: Girls
Percent of class, boys24.57*−58.179.17−36.412.15−96.16*−1.65−62.41*
(13.83)(69.85)(6.9)(33.22)(10.93)(53.17)(6.81)(32.59)
Percent of class, boys2−25.44*141.85−8.9583.22−5.8192.99*0.38123.22*
(13.32)(138)(6.67)(66.15)(10.48)(104.38)(6.57)(65.1)
Percent of class, boys3−108.9−59.99−129.4*−79.96*
(88.71)(42.86)(66.73)(42.31)
Calculated marginal effect of Boys−0.872.000.221.82−3.65−0.22−1.270.84
Wald test on joint significance (p-value)0.120.110.410.280.030.010.420.14
n14,28414,28414,28414,28414,28414,28414,28414,284
R20.860.860.860.860.850.850.860.86
Table 5

Table 5, sample broken out by gender, grade level percent boys as an instrument for classroom percent boys

Sample restricted to students in classrooms with greater than 20% boys and less than 80% boys, wave 2 only
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
Sample: Boys
Percent of class, boys−5.44*−11.02***−1.83−4.00
(2.90)(3.35)(2.72)(3.17)
First stage for percent boys in the class
Grade-level pct boys0.31***0.31***0.31***0.31***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
First stage diagnostics
F-stat830.67830.67829.84829.84
Partial R20.140.140.140.14
n5,1405,1405,1405,140
Sample: Girls
Percent of class, boys−2.55−1.80−1.86−1.43
(3.21)(3.29)(2.60)(3.18)
First stage for percent boys in the class
Grade-level pct boys0.31***0.31***0.31***0.31***
(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)(0.01)
First stage diagnostics
F-stat746.06746.06745.97745.97
Partial R20.130.130.130.13
n5,0405,0405,0405,040
Table 6

Corresponding estimates from Table 6, using the full sample, omitted category is 20–40% boys in the class

BoysGirls
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentileReading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
0–20%−1.83−3.921.56−2.341.925.61**3.55**3.12***
(3.45)(2.63)(2.35)(1.72)(6.90)(2.70)(1.47)(0.95)
40–60%−0.85*−0.21−0.220.070.02−0.04−0.71−0.48*
(0.51)(0.27)(0.40)(0.25)(0.57)(0.28)(0.45)(0.28)
60–80%−1.80***−0.78**−0.240.22−0.020.07−1.06**−0.64*
(0.68)(0.37)(0.53)(0.33)(0.68)(0.34)(0.53)(0.33)
80–100%−4.35*−2.271.321.320.20−2.05−1.13−1.95*
(2.62)(1.85)(1.68)(1.68)(2.17)(1.42)(1.27)(1.05)
n14,45814,45814,45814,45814,37414,37414,37414,374
R20.850.850.860.860.860.860.850.86
Table 7

Classroom gender composition, sub-samples by gender and fall kindergarten test score performance

Sample restricted to students in classrooms with greater than 20% boys and less than 80% boys
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
Sub-sample: Boys in top 50th percentile average Fall K scores
Percent boys in classroom−6.30**−1.50−0.320.55
(3.06)(1.51)(2.46)(1.39)
n6,8506,8506,8506,850
R20.880.870.890.88
Sub-sample: Boys in bottom 50th percentile average Fall K scores
Percent boys in classroom−4.66*−2.73**−2.09−0.36
(2.50)(1.37)(1.91)(1.26)
n7,5487,5487,5487,548
R20.860.880.860.88
Sub-sample: Girls in top 50th percentile average Fall K scores
Percent boys in classroom3.282.22*−2.74−0.84
(2.71)(1.26)(2.04)(1.15)
n7,5077,5077,5077,507
R20.880.880.880.88
Sub-sample: Girls in bottom 50th percentile average Fall K scores
Percent boys in classroom−3.05−1.54−3.59*−0.66
(2.51)(1.34)(1.92)(1.35)
n6,7776,7776,7776,777
R20.880.880.860.88
Table 8

Classroom gender composition interacted with an indicator for top 50th percentile in average Fall K scores

Sample restricted to students in classrooms with greater than 20% boys and less than 80% boys
Reading scoreReading percentileMath scoreMath percentile
Sub-sample: Boys
Percent boys in the classroom−3.04−2.75**0.65−0.07
(2.35)(1.31)(1.87)(1.24)
Student scored in top 50th percentile on Fall K scores9.98***4.68***5.91***3.31***
(1.66)(0.88)(1.35)(0.86)
(Percent boys) × (Student top 50th percentile)−5.76*1.59−4.28*0.53
(3.16)(1.71)(2.56)(1.67)
Linear combinations of effects:
Total effect of percent of boys for students in top 50th percentilea−8.80***−1.16−3.63*0.46
(2.71)(1.38)(2.19)(1.31)
n14,39814,39814,39814,398
R20.870.870.870.87
Sub-sample: Girls
Percent boys in the classroom−2.73−1.54−3.98**−2.18*
(2.39)(1.26)(1.90)(1.29)
Student scored in top 50th percentile on Fall K scores5.08***3.09***3.49***2.25***
(1.65)(0.84)(1.33)(0.86)
(Percent boys) × (Student top 50th percentile)2.883.48**0.532.03
(3.10)(1.57)(2.47)(1.61)
Linear combinations of effects:
Total effect of percent of boys for students in top 50th percentilea0.151.94−3.45*−0.15
(2.55)(1.22)(1.96)(1.19)
n14,28414,28414,28414,284
R20.880.880.880.88

References

Aizer, A. 2008. Peer Effects and Human Capital Accumulation: The Externalities of ADD. NBER Working Paper Series #14354. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.10.3386/w14354Search in Google Scholar

Ammermueller, A., and J.Pischke. 2009. “Peer Effects in European Primary Schools: Evidence from the Progress in International Reading Literacy Study.” Journal of Labor Economics27(3):315348.10.1086/603650Search in Google Scholar

Beckerman, T., and T.Good. 1981. “The Classroom Ratio of High- and Low-Aptitude Students and Its Effect on Achievement.” American Educational Research Journal18:31727.10.3102/00028312018003317Search in Google Scholar

Caldas, S. J., and C.Bankston III. 1997. “Effect of School Population Socioeconomic Status on Individual Academic Achievement.” Journal of Educational Research90:26977.10.1080/00220671.1997.10544583Search in Google Scholar

Caseau, D. L., R.Luckasson, and R. L.Kroth. 1994. “Special Education Services for Girls with Serious Emotional Disturbance: A Case of Gender Bias?Behavioral Disorders20(1):5160.10.1177/019874299402000101Search in Google Scholar

Cho, R. M. 2012. “Are There Peer Effects Associated with Having English Language Learner (ELL) Classmates? Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K).” Economics of Education Review31:62943.10.1016/j.econedurev.2012.04.006Search in Google Scholar

Coleman, J. S., E. Q.Campbell, C. J.Hobson, J.McPartland, A. M.Mood, F. D.Weinfeld, and R. L.York. 1966. Equality of Educational Opportunity. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.Search in Google Scholar

Currie, J. 2001. “Early Childhood Education Programs.” The Journal of Economic Perspectives15(2):21338.10.1257/jep.15.2.213Search in Google Scholar

Datar, A. 2006. “Does Delaying Kindergarten Entrance Give Children a Head Start?Economics of Education Review25(1):4362.10.1016/j.econedurev.2004.10.004Search in Google Scholar

Dee, T. S.2005. “A Teacher like Me: Does Race, Ethnicity, or Gender Matter?The American Economic Review95(2):158165.10.1257/000282805774670446Search in Google Scholar

Dee, T. S.2007. “Teachers and the Gender Gaps in Student Achievement.” The Journal of Human Resources42(3):528554.10.3368/jhr.XLII.3.528Search in Google Scholar

Figlio, D. N. 2007. “Boys Named Sue: Disruptive Children and Their Peers.” Education Finance and Policy2:37694.10.1162/edfp.2007.2.4.376Search in Google Scholar

Fletcher, J. 2010. “Spillover Effects of Inclusion of Classmates with Emotional Problems on Test Scores in Early Elementary School.” Journal of Policy Analysis and Management29(1):6983.10.1002/pam.20479Search in Google Scholar

Friend, J. 2006. “Research on Same-Gender Grouping in Eighth Grade Science Classrooms.” RMLE Online: Research in Middle Level Education30(4):115.10.1080/19404476.2006.11462037Search in Google Scholar

Gottfried, M. A. 2011. “Absent Peers in Elementary Years: The Negative Classroom Effects of Unexcused Absences on Standardized Testing Outcomes.” Teachers College Record, 113.Search in Google Scholar

Gottfried, M. A. 2012. “The Achievement Effects of Tardy Classmates: Evidence in Urban Elementary Schools.” School Effectiveness and School Improvement.10.1080/09243453.2012.728135Search in Google Scholar

Heckman, J. 2006. “Skill Formation and the Economics of Investing in Disadvantaged Children.” Science312(5782):19002.10.1126/science.1128898Search in Google Scholar

Henderson, V., P.Mieszkowski, and Y.Sauvageau. 1978. “Peer Group Effects and Educational Production Functions.” Journal of Public Economics10:97106.10.1016/0047-2727(78)90007-5Search in Google Scholar

Henry, G., and D.Rinkman. 2007. “Do Peers Influence Children’s Skill Development in Preschool?Economics of Education Review26(1):10012.10.1016/j.econedurev.2005.09.006Search in Google Scholar

Hoxby, C. M. 2000. “The Effect of Class Size on Student Achievement: New Evidence from Population Variation.” Quarterly Journal of Economics115:123985.10.1162/003355300555060Search in Google Scholar

Hubbard, L., and A.Datnow. 2005. “Do Single-Sex Schools Improve the Education of Low-Income and Minority Students? An Investigation of California’s Public Single-Gender Academies.” Anthropology and Education Quarterly36:11531.10.1525/aeq.2005.36.2.115Search in Google Scholar

Jackson, C. 2002. “Can Single-Sex Classes in Co-educational Schools Enhance the Learning Experiences of Girls and/or Boys? An Exploration of Pupils’ Perceptions.” British Educational Research Journal28:3748.10.1080/01411920120109739Search in Google Scholar

Juvonen, J., S.Graham, and M. A.Schuster. 2003. “Bullying Among Young Adolescents: The Strong, the Weak, and the Troubled.” Pediatrics112:123137.10.1542/peds.112.6.1231Search in Google Scholar

Lavy, V., and A.Schlosser. 2007. “Mechanisms and Impacts of Gender Peer Effects at School.” NBER Working Paper #13292.10.3386/w13292Search in Google Scholar

Lazear, E. 2001. “Educational Production.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics116:777803.10.1162/00335530152466232Search in Google Scholar

Leahey, E., and G.Guo. 2001. “Gender Differences in Mathematical Trajectories.” Social Forces80:713732.10.1353/sof.2001.0102Search in Google Scholar

Lee, V. E., and A. S.Bryk. 1986. “Effects of Single-Sex Secondary Schools on Student Achievement and Attitudes.” Journal of Educational Psychology78(5):381395.10.1037/0022-0663.78.5.381Search in Google Scholar

Lietz, J. J., and Gregory, M. K.1978. “Pupil Race and Sex Determinants of Office and Exceptional Education Referrals.” Educational Research Quarterly3(2):6166.Search in Google Scholar

Lindberg, S. M., J. S.Hyde, J. L.Petersen, and M. C.Linn. 2010. “New Trends in Gender and Mathematics Performance: A Meta-Analysis.” Psychological Bulletin136:112335.10.1037/a0021276Search in Google Scholar

Link, C. R., and J. G.Mulligan. 1991. “Classmates’ Effects on Black Student Achievement in Public School Classrooms.” Economics of Education Review10:297310.10.1016/0272-7757(91)90020-PSearch in Google Scholar

Marsh, H. W., and K. J.Rowe. 1996. “The Effects of Single-Sex and Mixed-Sex Mathematics Classes within a Coeducational School: A Reanalysis and Comment.” Australian Journal of Education4:14762.10.1177/000494419604000203Search in Google Scholar

Morse, S.1998. Separated by Sex: A critical Look at Single-Sex Education for Girls. The American Association of University Women Educational Foundation: Washington, DC.Search in Google Scholar

Mulligan, J. G. 1984. “A Classroom Production Function.” Economic Inquiry22:21826.10.1111/j.1465-7295.1984.tb00680.xSearch in Google Scholar

Mullis, I. V. S., M. O.Martin, and P.Foy. 2008. TIMSS 2007 International Report: Findings from IEA’s Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study at the Fourth and Eighth Grades. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS and PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College.Search in Google Scholar

Neidell, M., and J.Waldfogel. 2010. “Cognitive and Noncognitive Peer Effects in Early Education.” The Review of Economics and Statistics92:56276.10.1162/REST_a_00012Search in Google Scholar

Rennie, L., and L.Parker. 1997. “Students’ and Teachers’ Perceptions of Single-Sex and Mixed-Sex Mathematics Classes.” Mathematics Education Research Journal9:25773.10.1007/BF03217318Search in Google Scholar

Rowe, K. J., P. J.Nix, and G.Tepper. 1986. “Single-Sex and Mixed-Sex Classes: The Joint Effects of Gender and Class Type on Student Performance in the Attitudes towards Mathematics.” Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Australian Association for Research in Education, Melbourne, Australia.Search in Google Scholar

Sax, L. J., and C. E.Harper. 2007. “Origins of the Gender Gap: Pre-College and College Influences on Differences between Men and Women.” Research in Higher Education48:66994.10.1007/s11162-006-9046-zSearch in Google Scholar

Schneeweis, N., and M.Zweimueller. 2012. “Girls, Girls, Girls: Gender Composition and Female School Choice.” Economics of Education Review31(4):482500.10.1016/j.econedurev.2011.11.002Search in Google Scholar

Singh, N. N, C. R.Ellis, T. J.Landrum, L. S.Donatelli, and C.Hampton. 1994. “Characteristics Of Children And Adolescents With Serious Emotional Disturbance In Systems Of Care. Part I: Partial Hospitalization And Inpatient Psychiatric ServicesJournal of Emotional and Behavior Disorders2(1):1320.10.1177/106342669400200102Search in Google Scholar

Skiba, R. J., R. L.Peterson, and T.Williams. 1997. “Office Referrals and Suspension: Disciplinary Intervention in Middle Schools.” Education and Treatment of Children20(3):295315.Search in Google Scholar

Spencer, S. J., C. M.Steele, and D. M.Quinn. 1999. “Stereotype Threat and Women’s Math Performance.” Social Forces35:428.10.1006/jesp.1998.1373Search in Google Scholar

Summers, A., and B.Wolfe. 1978. “Do Schools Make a Difference?American Economic Review67:63952.Search in Google Scholar

U.S. Department of Education (USDOE), 2005. Office of Planning, Evaluation and Policy Development Policy and Program Studies Service, Single-Sex Versus Secondary Schooling: A Systematic Review, Washington, D.C.Search in Google Scholar

Vosniadou, S.2001. How Children Learn, Educational Practices Series, 7, The International Academy of Education (IAE) and the International Bureau of Education (UNESCO).Search in Google Scholar

Wagner, M., L.Newman, R.D’Amico, E. D.Jay, P.Butler-Nalin, C.Marder, et al.1991. Youth with Disabilities: How Are They Doing?Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.Search in Google Scholar

Whitmore, D. 2005. “Resource and Peer Impacts on Girls’ Academic Achievement: Evidence from a Randomized Experiment.” American Economic Review95:199203.10.1257/000282805774670158Search in Google Scholar

Willms, J. D. 1986. “Social Class Segregation and Its Relationship to Pupils’ Examination Results in Scotland.” American Sociological Review51:22441.10.2307/2095518Search in Google Scholar

Zimmer, R. W., and E. F.Toma. 2000. “Peer Effects in Private and Public Schools across Countries.” Journal of Policy Analysis and Management19:7592.10.1002/(SICI)1520-6688(200024)19:1<75::AID-PAM5>3.0.CO;2-WSearch in Google Scholar

  1. 1

    For example, Figlio (2007) studies the effects of having classmates with behavioral issues, Cho (2012) studies effects of having English Language Learner classmates and Gottfried (2011, 2012) studies the effect of having truant or tardy classmates.

  2. 2

    Separately examined nonlinear effects by gender can also inform a larger literature on gender gaps in academic achievement. For instance, there have been long-standing concerns over female students’ achievement on math and science (Lindberg et al. 2010; Mullis, Martin, and Foy 2008), and more recent concerns over lagging reading performance and lower college attendance rates for males (Sax and Harper 2007).

  3. 3

    Hoxby (2000) and Lavy and Schlosser (2007) find that more females in the classroom positively impact both genders in both math and reading. Whitmore (2005), also studying younger elementary school students, as we do here, finds different effects by gender. However, she uses the average percentile rank in both math and reading and does not explore the possibility of subject-specific effects.

  4. 4

    For good reviews of the literature see USDOE (2005) and Morse (1998).

  5. 5

    In the ECLS-K data, information was first collected from kindergartners (as well as parents, teachers and school administrators) from approximately 1,000 kindergarten programs in both the fall and spring of the 1998–1999 school year. This is a panel study where the initial sample has currently been followed up through grade 8, with data follow-up collection on the full sample in the spring of grades 1, 3, 5 and 8. There is very little turnover between the kindergarten and first-grade waves, with a somewhat higher rate of turnover between the first- and third-grade waves (Fletcher 2010; Cho 2012).

  6. 6

    The IRT scale scores represent estimates of the number of items students would have answered correctly if they had answered all possible questions on the standardized tests in both reading and math.

  7. 7

    We also drew multiple iterations of a random sample of students from both boy and girl samples in order to conduct a test of mean differences with a smaller sample. The t-statistics based on this random sampling algorithm are not significant – there are no remaining differences between the gender samples for the descriptive statistics reported in Table 1.

  8. 8

    This descriptive evidence can be found in Appendix Table 1.

  9. 9

    These variables include a private school indicator, enrollment size and percent minority at the school level. A total of 32 schools switch between public and private school status (1% of all schools), 593 schools experience a change in enrollment size (20% of all schools) and 523 schools experience a change in minority percentage (17% of all schools) in the sample across waves. The inclusion or exclusion of these measures from regressions using school-fixed effects does not greatly alter estimated effects or any conclusions from results.

  10. 10

    In addition to the model presented in the tables, we also tested the robustness of our estimation strategy with the following ancillary models: (1) 1-wave lagged measure of testing ability instead of a fall kindergarten baseline measure; (2) Interaction between 1-wave lagged measure and dummy variable for survey wave; and (3) Interaction between kindergarten baseline achievement score and dummy variable for survey wave. The results from these analyses do not differ from those presented in the article. They are available upon request.

  11. 11

    To account for repeat observations on the same individuals, we have also estimated the same specifications (clustered at the classroom level) presented in this article, when alternatively clustered at the individual level. Significance of results is not changed much by doing so. Additionally, in some specifications, estimation is done separately by wave, with findings supportive of result from the pooled sample.

  12. 12

    For example, the Wald test rejects that the coefficients for percent boys in the classroom between 30% and 40% and between 60% and 70% are equal with an F-statistic of 5.08 and p-value of 0.0242.

  13. 13

    The marginal effects of the percent boys in the classroom are reported in Appendix Table 4, along with the p-value from a Wald test for the joint significance of the main effect of the percent boys, the quadratic and cubic terms. The marginal effects are consistent with the main effects of the article in terms of general magnitude, and the p-values from the Wald tests show joint significance for the effect of a higher percent boys in the classroom on boys reading performance and girls math performance.

  14. 14

    While not reported here, estimates are nearly identical when different ways of controlling for student ability are used. See footnote 10.

  15. 15

    In addition to estimation with interactions that use a student performing above the 50th percentile in average fall kindergarten test score performance, we also ran estimation with alternative ways of categorizing prior student ability. We ran regressions instead using indicators broken out by quartiles and terciles, finding the same general results as presented here. In each case, results found negative reading effects for boys and not for girls, and negative math effects for girls, but not for boys. The larger negative reading effects for relatively high prior ability boys also remain evident in these additional specifications.

Published Online: 2014-2-1
Published in Print: 2014-7-1

©2014 by Walter de Gruyter Berlin / Boston

Downloaded on 19.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/bejeap-2013-0123/html
Scroll to top button