ExLibris header image
SFX Logo
Title: Touch can change visual slant perception
Source:

Nature Neuroscience [1097-6256] Ernst, Marc yr:2000


Collapse list of basic services Basic
Full text
Full text available via Nature
GO
Document delivery
Request document via Library/Bibliothek GO
Users interested in this article also expressed an interest in the following:
1. Prinz, W. "Perception and action planning." The European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9.2 (1997): 129-154. Link to Full Text for this item Link to SFX for this item
2. Ernst, Marc O O. "Merging the senses into a robust percept." Trends in cognitive sciences 8.4 (2004): 162-9. Link to SFX for this item
3. Watt, J. "The visual control of reaching and grasping : Binocular disparity and motion parallax." Journal of experimental psychology. human perception and performance 29.2 (2003): 404-415. Link to SFX for this item
4. Rivest, J. "Depth perception as a function of motion parallax and absolute-distance information." Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance 12.3 (1986): 331-7. Link to SFX for this item
5. SHAMS, L. "What you see is what you hear." Nature 408.6814 (2000): 788-788. Link to Full Text for this item Link to SFX for this item
6. Hommel, B. "The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning." The Behavioral and brain sciences 24.05 (2002): 849-78. Link to SFX for this item
7. Spector, F. "Synesthesia : A new approach to understanding the development of perception." Developmental psychology 45.1 (2009): 175-189. Link to SFX for this item
8. Flach, R. "Predicting the effects of actions: Interactions of perception and action." Psychological science 12.6 (2001): 467-472. Link to Full Text for this item Link to SFX for this item
9. Ernst, Marc O O. "Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion." Nature 415.6870 (2002): 429-433. Link to Full Text for this item Link to SFX for this item
10. Hillis, J. M. "Combining Sensory Information: Mandatory Fusion Within, but Not Between, Senses." Science 298.5598 (2002): 1627-1630. Link to SFX for this item
11. ROCK, I. "VISION AND TOUCH." Scientific American 216.5 (1967): 96-104. Link to SFX for this item
12. Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne J. "From the perception of action to the understanding of intention." Nature reviews. Neuroscience 2.8 (2001): 561-7. Link to Full Text for this item Link to SFX for this item
13. Posner, M I I. "Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance." Psychological review 83.2 (1976): 157-71. Link to SFX for this item
14. Adelson, Edward H. "Single lens stereo with a plenoptic camera." IEEE transactions on pattern analysis and machine intelligence 14.2 (1992): 99-106. Link to SFX for this item
15. Veraart, C. "Changes in occipital cortex activity in early blind humans using a sensory substitution device." Brain Research 826.1 (1999): 128-34. Link to Full Text for this item Link to SFX for this item
16. Goldman, A. "Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading." Trends in cognitive sciences 2.12: 493-501. Link to SFX for this item
17. Bargh, J A A. "The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction." Journal of personality and social psychology 76.6: 893-910. Link to SFX for this item
18. Motoki, T. "Present status of three-dimensional television research." Proceedings of the IEEE 83.7 (1995): 1009-1021. Link to SFX for this item
19. Rogers, B. "Motion parallax as an independent cue for depth perception: a retrospective." Perception 38.6 (2009): 907-11. Link to SFX for this item
20. Braunstein, M L L. "Shape and depth perception from parallel projections of three-dimensional motion." Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance 10.6 (1984): 749-60. Link to SFX for this item
View More...
View Less...
Select All Clear All

Expand list of advanced services Advanced