Skip to main content
Log in

Preferences, beliefs, and values in negotiations concerning aid to Nicaragua

  • Published:
Public Choice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The paper argues that in the context of public choice for non-market goods, two assumptions of the simple model of the rational economic actor may not hold. The assumptions are that there is a direct connection between choice and outcome, and that preferences are not affected by the act of making a choice. Consequently, to understand people's preferences for public goods, it is important to measure their beliefs and values separately rather than simply to observe their choice behavior or to ask them what they would be willing to pay for the public good. In an example study, people's preferences for U.S. policies toward Nicaragua were measured and further analyzed into their beliefs about the effects of those policies on Nicaraguan outcomes, and their evaluations of the Nicaraguan outcomes. It was shown that the process of making a two-person choice changed the preferences, and that the separate measures or beliefs and values gave insight into the process of the change that would not have been available had only the preferences been measured. Implications for the contingent valuation method are explored and an alternative approach is proposed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, N.H. and Zalinski, J. (1988). Functional measurement approach to self-estimation in multi-attribute evaluation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 1: 191–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balke, W.M., Hammond, K.R. and Meyer, G.D. (1973). An alternative approach to labor-management negotiations. Administrative Science Quarterly 18: 311–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, R.R., McClelland, G.H., Schulze, W.D., Brown, T.C. and Peterson, G.L. (1990). An experimental examination of intrinsic environmental values. University of Colorado.

  • Cohen, J. and Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coursey, D.L., Hovis, J.L. and Schulze, W.D. (1987). The disparity between willingness to accept andd willingness to pay measures of value. Quarterly Journal of Economics (August): 679–690.

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R.H. (in press). Rethinking rational choice. In R. Friedland and A.F. Robertson (Eds.), Beyond the marketplace: Rethinking economy and society. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

  • Goldstein, W.M. and Einhorn, H.J. (1987). Expression theory and the preference reversal phenomenon. Psychological Review 94: 236–254.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gluck, R. (1975). An economic evaluation of the Rakaia Fishery as a recreation resource (Monograph No. 6). Melbourne: Monash University, Australian Recreation Research Association. (Cited in Sinden and Worrell, 1979.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Grether, D.M. and Plott, C.R. (1979). Economic theory of choice and the preference reversal phenomenon. American Economic Review 69: 623–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamm, R.M. (1979). The conditions of occurrence of the preference reversal phenomenon. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University. (University Microfilms No. 80-12829.)

  • Hamm, R.M., Bennett, L., Wunderlich, K. and Howe, C. (1990). Stability and interpretation of survey respondents' unwillingness to pay for increases, or accept compensation for decreases, in water supply reliability. Boulder, CO: Environment and Behavior Program, Institute of Behavioral Sciences, University of Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamm, R.M., Miller, M.A. and Ling, R.S. (1989). Cognitive feedback in negotiations (Publication No. 89–1). Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado, Boulder.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, K.R. and Adelman, L. (1976). Science, values, and human judgment. Science 194: 389–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, K.R., Stewart, T.R., Brehmer, B. and Steinmann, D.O. (1975). Social judgment theory. In M.F. Kaplan and S. Schwartz (Eds.), Human judgment and decision processes, 271–312. New York: Academic Press. Reprinted in H.R. Arkes and K.R. Hammond (Eds.), Judgment and decision making: An interdisciplinary reader, 56–76. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1986.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haveman, R.H. and Knopf, K.A. (1970). The market system, 2nd Edition. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1986). Comments on the contingent valuation method. In R.G. Cummings, D.S. Brookshire and W.D. Shultze (Eds.), Valuing environmental goods. Totawa, NJ: Rowman and Allanheld.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. and Knetsch, J.L. (1990). Valuing public goods: The purchase of moral satisfaction. Paper presented at meeting of the Public Choice Society, Tucson, Arizona, 16–18 March.

  • Lichtenstein, S. and Slovic, P. (1971). Reversals of preference between bids and choices in gambling decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 89: 46–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodge, M. (1981). Magnitude scaling: Quantitative measurement of opinions. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milter, R.G. and Rohrbaugh, J. (1988). Judgment analysis and decision conferencing for administrative review: A case study of innovative policy making in government. In R.L. Cardy, S.M. Puffer and J.M. Newman (Eds.), Advances in information processing in organizations, 245–262. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, R.C. and Carson, R.T. (1989). Using surveys to value public goods: The contingent valuation method. Washington, DC: Resources for the Future.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E. and Wilson, T.D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84: 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roose, J.E. and Doherty, M.E. (1976). Judgment theory applied to the selection of life insurance salesmen. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 16: 231–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberman, J. and Klock, M. (1989). The behavior of respondents in contingent valuation: Evidence on starting bids. Journal of Behavioral Economics 18: 51–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinden, J.A. and Worrell, A.C. (1979). Unpriced values: Decisions without market prices. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, T.R. (1988). Judgment analysis: Procedures. In B. Brehmer and C.R.B. Joyce (Eds.), Human judgment: The SJT approach, 41–74. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, T.R. and Ely, D.W. (1984). Range sensitivity: A necessary condition and a test for the validity of weights (NCAR 3141–84/14). Boulder CO: National Center for Atmospheric Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, P.A. (1989). A theory of causal processing. British Journal of Psychology 80: 431–454.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The research was partially supported by the Small Grant Program of the Conflict Resolution Consortium, University of Colorado, Boulder, which in turn was funded by the Hewlett Foundation. Dr. Hamm was a National Research Council Senior Research Associate at the Army Research Institute Field Unit, Fort Leavenworth, Kansas, during part of the project. David Bartos helped run subjects and analyze data. Assistance finding subjects was provided by Major Steve Whitworth and Cadet Rosita Bachman of C.U. Army ROTC and by Professor Philip Langer. Assistance in evaluating the realism of the stimulus materials was provided by Martha Gibson, John Paul Lederach, and Professor Joel Edelstein.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hamm, R.M., Miller, M.A. & Ling, R.S. Preferences, beliefs, and values in negotiations concerning aid to Nicaragua. Public Choice 74, 79–103 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00175212

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00175212

Keywords

Navigation